Archives
January 2025
Categories
All
|
Back to Blog
Lighting for reassurance1/1/2025 Image credit: Gerd Altmann 2015 words / 8-minute read We've all been there at some point. You're walking in a dark space at night, alone, maybe to your car in a parking lot. And a kind of fear seizes you. You can't see very well around you, so you don't know if there is someone waiting in the shadows. You get to where you're going as fast as you can, unlock your car door, get in and lock the door behind you. For a moment, you feel safe again. My friend and colleague Nancy Clanton, a lighting designer based in Colorado, explains how the situation is even more acute for women. She told me about an incident once, some years ago, when she was in an unfamiliar city to attend a lighting conference. She had to walk alone at night along a dark stretch of road between the convention center and her hotel. Even with all her knowledge about lighting and darkness, she still felt uneasy being out there by herself. She saw two people, both silhouettes, approaching. Like many people in the same situation, Nancy had to make a quick decision. Were they two threatening men? An innocuous couple? Something else? In her mind, at that moment, was born the idea of "lighting for reassurance". With a small amount of well placed light, she could have made a quick friend-or-foe determination. And if circumstances required, she could have made a quick exit. It would have changed the situation in a fundamental way. Why do we light outdoor spaces at night?There are many reasons that people use artificial light at night. For example, we light sidewalks and pathways to help people orient and find their way from Point A to Point B. We light roadways and "conflict zones" where different kinds of traffic come together, because we know that doing so saves lives. We like to add nighttime amenity to outdoor spaces, making them more inviting for activities like commerce. But we also liked it because many people believe that light at night deters or even prevents the incidence of crime. Does outdoor lighting yield any real, positive influence on nighttime safety and security? Does it reduce criminal behavior? "The influence of outdoor light at night on crime is mixed," writes DarkSky International in its most recent annual report Artificial Light at Night: State of the Science 2024. "Some of the same studies that looked at lighting and traffic/pedestrian safety also considered nighttime crime incidence. Certain studies reported crime reduction when lighting is added to outdoor spaces. Others find either a negative effect, no effect, or mixed results." Whatever the ways in which lighting and crime interact, it's not like the effect of administering a drug to a person. That is, there is no sense that a dose X of the medicine always produces effect Y in the individual. How outdoor lighting and crime interact (or don't)Crime and lighting seem to have a very context-dependent relationship. There are very many of what researchers call "confounding variables". These are influences unrelated to the study subject that might produce some kind of effect in the system. Without careful control of these variables, the researcher can draw an incorrect conclusion on the basis of observing the intended effect. To continue the medical analogy, drug researchers try to remove differences among people in patient study samples. These include age, gender, geographic, location, and other factors that might produce a result that mimics what the drug should do. Failure to deal with these "confounders" means that one can be fooled into believing there is an effect from a drug that is in fact, unrelated to its mechanism of action. It is also very difficult to carry out high-quality, well-controlled studies. Designing reliable (and replicable) lighting studies is challenging. There is not much research funding for this. Sometimes the sources of research funding are questionable. Researchers worry they may want to see some sort of pre-defined result from studies they support. And there are instances where experimental design is just bad. For example, a 2019 study conducted in New York City housing projects claimed a strong relationship between high levels of light and decreased crime. The study looked at the effect of placing intense light sources on portable towers outside housing buildings and tracking changes in the incidence of crime in the immediate environment around them. To the surprise of few, crime dropped. But that's what happens when you treat people like criminals and light up their homes like they were prison yards: you get unreliable results. More than just fear of the darkWhat underlies concerns about outdoor lighting, public safety and crime is the unspoken element of fear. It is a powerful and underestimated motivator of human behavior. Our evolutionary heritage has given us limited abilities to see at night. As diurnal animals are active during the day, it made night a dangerous place for our early human ancestors. Although we tend to think of fear of the dark as something that particularly afflicts children, many people as adults feel a deep-seated sense of terror toward dark places at night. How much we know about a place determines our sense of ease (or unease) about it. Because we are so dependent on our sense of sight, it's easy to feel that we lose control when we cannot see what is happening around us. Psychologists have long understood that these influences have tremendous power in shaping our perception of the world. In recent years, social scientists have begun investigating what they call "feelings of safety". That is, they try to measure the degree to which people feel safe or unsafe in different situations that may involve danger. It is important to know that feelings of safety are independent of whether people are actually safe. There are many instances in which people feel unsafe about a situation but the evidence shows that there was no actual danger. There are ways in which feelings of safety can increase in dark places at night that lead people to accept lower light levels. This relates to a sense in which people feel trapped by their surroundings. They are looking for the exits, so to speak, in case danger suddenly emerges. Research shows that lower light levels are less acceptable to most people in any situation where they feel trapped. If there are clear opportunities to escape if needed, the acceptability of lower light levels begins to rise. Other work shows that most people prefer somewhat higher levels of outdoor light at night. Most prefer light that is cooler than warmer in color appearance. But the value of this extra light diminishes very quickly as light intensities increase. The relationship between feelings of safety and light intensity appears to be logarithmic. What that means is that as light levels increase, the amount of an increase necessary to produce some certain amount of increase in feelings of safety becomes larger. The biggest increases in feelings of safety happen in moving from situations where there is no light to those in which there is a very small amount of light. To raise feelings of safety by again as much requires increasing the light intensity by much more, often more than a factor of ten. Figure 7 from Svechkina et al. (2020). In this study, researchers asked subjects in three Israeli cities about their feelings concerning outdoor spaces under different intensities of light at night. Their models for feelings of safety (FoS) as a function of light intensity (solid lines) show logarithmic increases. The increase in feelings of safety flattens out at highlight levels. We don't quite understand why. One theory is that high intensity lighting makes people feel insecure by promoting the sense that they are on display. Bright lighting can create deep, dark shadows between objects that obscure those spaces. A person moving through an outdoor space at night may not be able to tell whether there is a threat hiding in those shadows. The glare from very intense lighting sources also has a disabling effect on the viewer. Glare causes the pupil of the eye to contract, which reduces the depth of field of vision. It conveys a distinct disadvantage to the viewer. And it's the same "prison yard lighting" effect in the New York study mentioned earlier. Rethinking how we light the nighttime worldThat's where "lighting for reassurance" comes in. It's an outdoor lighting ethic still new in the design community. While traditional design holds that more is better, lighting for reassurance asserts that better is better. Often that's less light chosen with a strategy in mind. The goal is to improve outdoor visibility at night and help users of spaces discern where threats might be. In particular, it makes use of the incredible properties of human vision, even at low light intensities, to see very small changes in contrast. It ensures that all potential safety hazards are clearly indicated. It also means not going overboard with lighting levels to the point that it becomes a disability for the viewer. It also makes outdoor spaces at night more inviting to other users of those spaces. People generally tend to feel more unsafe when they are alone in such places at night. As more people fill outdoor squares, streets and other places, they begin to feel more safe in the presence of a crowd. This couples with the popularity of using outdoor light at night for aesthetic purposes. This can include lighting of building façades, statues and monuments, and other landscape features to enhance the sense of nighttime placemaking. And it avoids creating glare to the greatest practical extent. The task is now to communicate this to more lighting designers. They don't usually learn it either in their formal education or as part of their praxis. We also have to change how lighting designers and engineers think about the concepts of minimum and maximum lighting levels. Right now, lighting standards generally tend to indicate only minimum lighting levels for various lighting applications. This is a belief, not always supported by evidence, that only certain minimum values are "safe". But it is often not clear why those values are "safe" while other values are not. It also does not take into account the idea that lighting can be too bright in some cases. That can create its own security problems. Organizations that make outdoor lighting standards are just beginning to embrace the notion that along with recommended minimum lighting values should come corresponding maximum values. The right lighting levels would make Goldilocks herself feel safe at night because they're neither too low or too high. They're just right. Reassurance for better nightsWhat can we learn from all this? For one thing, many people experience fear of nighttime darkness, and that fear is visceral. We shouldn't tell them that they are wrong to feel fear because we have data that somehow show that their fears are unfounded.
It's also the case that feelings of safety are very powerful. We should recognize this and leverage that fact in outdoor lighting design. Furthermore, the human eye is an underrated detector of faint light at night. That said, lighting design often does not fully exploit its amazing properties. Instead, and as lighting has become much cheaper than ever to consume, design has pushed light levels very high. The result is that the way we do lighting design now often does more harm than good, even though its practitioners clearly want to do good. What we can do for them is to help them put in place better design through lighting for reassurance. It combines the best of what scientists tell us about lighting with the power of psychology. Together, we can make outdoor spaces at night that are truly more safe while also helping people in them feel safer. If we overcome the fear that now leads to demand for brighter outdoor spaces, we'll increase support for measures that gradually draw down light pollution. To do so would be a win for all involved: for people, for the environment, and for the night sky.
0 Comments
Read More
Back to Blog
Image credit: Cottonbro Studio 1320 words / 5-minute read Summary: Although subject to widely held folk beliefs, the truth about how outdoor lighting and crime interact is very unsettled scientifically. This post examines the evidence, identifies shortcomings in current research and practice, and suggests the factors and considerations that really matter. Imagine that you are in an unfamiliar city, far from home. You're walking along a street that doesn't have much lighting, passing people you can't see very well. What kind of feeling does it cause you? For many people, it isn't a good one; for women in particular, the sense of fear can be crippling. Now imagine what would ease that fear. More lighting? How bright? What if it were as bright as daylight? How much light would it take to reassure you that it was safe to walk there? These are questions people have been asking for almost as long as there has been artificial lighting. Many people, at least in Western cultures, think that an association between darkness and crime is a given. This is in part built on folk beliefs that draw direct parallels: dark = bad, light = good. It's become part of our folklore, a kind of received wisdom that few question. But what does science say about all this? Is there an evidence-based case for lighting up the night in the name of safety? And what does it tell us about how we light the world now? Evidence in disarrayThe evidence about lighting crime is very unsettled. In its Artificial Light at Night: State of the Science 2023 report, DarkSky International puts it this way: "Certain studies reported crime reduction when lighting is added to outdoor spaces. [1] Others find either a negative effect, [2] no effect, [3-4] or mixed results. [5]" There is no obvious and consistent relationship between outdoor lighting and crime that shows up in the data. Rather, whether an experiment shows a positive or negative association between them depends very much on the details. We can then ask: why is the picture so unclear? For one thing, it's difficult to get lighting and crime studies funded. When funds are available, they sometimes come from lighting equipment manufacturers. While this does not itself render the results unreliable, it demands unusual transparency from researchers to avoid any hint of bias. Some studies are not subjected to peer review, instead appearing in the so-called 'grey literature'. And some are conducted outside the parameters of what are considered best research practices, such as pre-registration of trials. This makes it very difficult to interpret results of valid experiments and compare them against each other. Are the results reproducible? And would anyone hang their hat on this evidence? In fairness to the research community, it's difficult to design and conduct valid experiments. That's because crime is a human behavior subject to a complex psychology. The result is that many variables influence crime beyond the time of day and whether light is present at night. Studies on lighting and fear of crime often can't distinguish lighting from other effects. For example, in one study that considered how the intensity of lighting influenced public perceptions of outdoor spaces at night, the authors admitted that "if a location feels unsafe in daylight then it is likely to feel unsafe after dark, regardless of the light level." [6] And how much of this is real relative to what we know about crime incidence? In a problem that "seems reasonably unique to crime," [7] there is a significant difference in terms of how prevalent people think crime is relative to its actual prevalence. [8] People predisposed to expect crime in certain areas will likely never find satisfaction in the lighting conditions in those places. The bottom line is that almost all lighting and crime results are suspect to some extent. "Studies which purport to show large lighting benefit for public safety tend to be of poor scientific and statistical quality, done by those with poor scientific and statistical background," says Dr. Paul Marchant, a statistician and researcher based in the UK who has published extensively on the subject. "[Even] better quality, large temporal- and spatial-scale studies are unable to detect any public safety lighting benefit.” "Feelings of safety" and lighting for "reassurance"Another way of looking at the problem is to focus less on testing whether lighting deters or prevents crime and more on the effect it has on the observer. What makes people feel safe in outdoor spaces at night? Feelings of insecurity are very powerful and drive people toward high light levels because they "feel safe" in the same outdoor spaces during the daytime. Make no mistake: the feeling of fear is real and, for many people, visceral. [9] And for many of the same people, they think that they won't be safe unless outdoor lighting levels are like daylight. That implies light as a crime deterrent, based on the assumption that criminals are less likely to act if they think they will be seen by others. Even if the basis for that mechanism isn't true, it can have a profound effect on users of outdoor spaces at night. Researchers have considered the notion of "feelings of safety" (FoS) as a measurable quantity. In 2020, Alina Svechkina, Tamar Trop and Boris Portnov of the University of Haifa in Israel put volunteers into nighttime spaces in several Israeli cities. They varied the lighting conditions and then asked the volunteers to rate their sense of safety and security under each lighting treatment. Their results suggest that FoS rise the fastest with increasing lighting levels when going from no light to low intensity. [10] But FoS rapidly levels out when going toward higher light levels. In other words, it's a classic case of "diminishing returns": doubling the amount of light doesn't double FoS. Worse, the application of bright lighting in one area might have the result of moving crime around. Lisa Tompson (University of Waikato, New Zealand, and University College London) and coworkers found that the absence of street lighting on city streets in the UK "may prevent theft from vehicles, but there is a danger of offenses being temporally or spatially displaced." In other words, thieves might just move their nefarious activities to adjacent (and more brightly-lit) neighborhoods, or from overnight hours to the daytime. To reduce dependence on nighttime lighting in the interest of reducing light pollution while also ensuring public support for such measures, some lighting designers are turning to the notion of "reassurance" lighting. This kind of design targets lighting to tasks like recognition of faces and expressions while emphasizing uniformity and object detection. [11] What can we do?Fear of crime is real, and we should pay attention to it. Throwing facts and figures at people who experience that fear is unlikely to change their perception. Instead, the use of "lighting for reassurance" practices shows some promise. Surprisingly small amounts of light are useful for reassurance. But high light levels can make people feel like they're in a literal spotlight, compromising feelings of safety and making people feel more vulnerable. So what's the right answer? Bearing in mind that there is no magical formula that predicts the "right" lighting parameters, there are useful takeaways in all these points:
Marchant says, "It is hardly surprising that human beings are uneasy about the dark as we are a daytime species," while noting that nocturnal species likely feel just as uneasy about their existence in a world increasingly lit with artificial light at night. However, "[our] unease does not mean that a less brightly lit night is dangerous in terms of both crime and road traffic collisions." Using the best of what we know from science and practice can help create outdoor spaces at night that are not only beautiful and functional, but also empowering to users. References
|